Top-Rated Free Essay
Preview

Fthe Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis

Powerful Essays
2260 Words
Grammar
Grammar
Plagiarism
Plagiarism
Writing
Writing
Score
Score
Fthe Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis
fThe Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis
(Wikipedia Entry) 'He gave man speech, and speech created thought, Which is the measure of the universe ' - Prometheus Unbound, Shelley The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis as we know it today can be broken down into two basic principles: linguistic determinism and linguistic relativity.

Linguistic Determinism: A Definition
Linguistic Determinism refers to the idea that the language we use to some extent determines the way in which we view and think about the world around us. The concept has generally been divided into two separate groups - 'strong ' determinism and 'weak ' determinism. Strong determinism is the extreme version of the theory, stating that language actually determines thought, that language and thought are identical. Although this version of the theory would attract few followers today - since it has strong evidence against it, including the possibility of translation between languages - we will see that in the past this has not always been the case. Weak determinism, however, holds that thought is merely affected by or influenced by our language, whatever that language may be. This version of determinism is widely accepted today.

Wilhelm von Humboldt: The 'Weltanschauung ' Hypothesis.
Wilhelm von Humboldt (1767-1835) was the first European to combine a knowledge of various languages with a philosophical background; he equated language and thought exactly in a hypothesis we now call the 'Weltanschauung ' (world-view) hypothesis, in fact a version of the extreme form of the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis. Humboldt maintained that language actually determined thought: Der mensch lebt mit den Gegenständen hauptsächlich, ja...sogar ausschliesslich so, wie die Sprache sie ihm zuführt." Humboldt viewed thought as being impossible without language, language as completely determining thought. On closer inspection, we can see that this extreme hypothesis leads to a question: how, if there was no thought before language, did language arise in the first place? Humboldt answers this by adhering to the theory that language is a platonic object, comparable to a living organism which just suddenly evolved one day entirely of its own accord.

Linguistic Relativity: A Definition
Linguistic relativity states that distinctions encoded in one language are unique to that language alone, and that "there is no limit to the structural diversity of languages". If one imagines the colour spectrum, it is a continuum, each colour gradually blending into the next; there are no sharp boundaries. But we impose boundaries; we talk of red, orange, yellow, green, blue, indigo, and violet. It takes little thought to realise that these discriminations are arbitrary - and indeed in other languages the boundaries are

different. In neither Spanish, Italian nor Russian is there a word that corresponds to the English meaning of 'blue ', and likewise in Spanish there are two words 'esquina ' and 'rincon ', meaning an inside and an outside corner, which necessitate the use of more than one word in English to convey the same concept. These examples show that the language we use, whichever it happens to be, divides not only the colour spectrum, but indeed our whole reality, which is a 'kaleidoscopic flux of impressions ', into completely arbitrary compartments.

The Notion of Codability
Codability has been defined by Peter Herriot as 'the ease with which a language tag can be used to distinguish one item from another '. Something is codable if it falls within the scope of readily available terms used in whatever particular language. Degrees of codability vary, in that while one language may be capable of expressing a concept with just one word, in another may be necessary to use a whole phrase to get across the same notion; a famous example of this is the fact that in Eskimo there are many different words for snow, depending on which kind of snow one is talking about. If we are looking for evidence to prove the weak version of linguistic determinism, then we need look no further than various experiments that have been conducted around codability. For example, monolingual speakers of an American-Indian language called Zuni - a language which does not recognise any difference between yellow and orange - had more difficulty in re-identifying objects of such colours after a period of time. With monolingual English speakers, this difficulty is absent, since we make a verbal distinction. This only offers support for the weak version of the hypothesis, though, because it would be wrong to say that the Zuni speakers did not actually perceive a difference. So the more highly codable a concept is, the easier it is to retrieve from the unconscious. This we will come back to later when considering the relationship between a Freudian theory and linguistic determinism.

The Notion of Translatability
Closely related to the notion of codability is the notion of translatability. Although different languages may have different ways of dividing up their spectra of experience into verbal forms, we find it is still quite possible to translate from one language into another. Although someone translating from one language into another may find it necessary to use a whole phrase in the target language to communicate the concept expressed in the original language with only a single word, this is achievable. In the Australian aboriginal language Pinupti, the word 'katarta ' refers to the hole left by a goanna when it has broken the surface of its burrow after hibernation. It takes seventeen words to translate that concept into English, but the result is fine, lacking perhaps some of the conciseness but none of the subtlety of the Pinupti word. Of course inter-language translatability again offers evidence against the strong version of determinism. The differences between the lexicons of individuals would carry great import. I know the meaning of the word 'saltatoria '; the person sitting next to me word-processing a dissertation on paediatrics would probably not know the meaning of it. This does not, of course, mean that I would be unable to explain to him what it meant. Of course another thing to bear in mind is the fact that words are often borrowed from one language into another, for instance the French borrowing 'le weekend ' from English. This sort of borrowing would be impossible if language determined thought completely. And if we look just a little further, it becomes obvious that if it was true that language dictated thought, and that concepts were untranslatable, then children would be incapable of learning language at all; for how would a child learn its first word?

Edward Sapir and Benjamin Lee Whorf 'Human beings do not live in the objective world alone, nor alone in the world of social activity as ordinarily understood, but are very much at the mercy of the particular language which has become the medium of expression for their society. It is quite an illusion to imagine that one adjusts to reality essentially without the use of language and that language is merely an incidental means of solving specific problems of communication and reflection. The fact of the matter is that the "real world" is to a large extent unconsciously built up on the language habits of the group. ' This famous passage from the American linguist and anthropologist Edward Sapir (1884-1936) 's 'The Status Of Linguistics As A Science ', written in 1929, demonstrates the dominating thought of what has come to be called by all sorts of names including the 'Sapir-Whorf hypothesis ', the 'Whorfian hypothesis ' and more plainly the 'Linguistic Relativity hypothesis '. We can see the reason for the variety of titles for the hypothesis - as well as the influence Sapir must have had on his pupil Benjamin Lee Whorf (1897-1941) - if we look at the following passage from Whorf himself, which propounds much the same viewpoint: 'We dissect nature along lines laid down by our native languages. The categories and types that we isolate from the world of phenomena we do not find there because they stare every observer in the face; on the contrary, the world is presented in a kaleidoscopic flux of impressions which has to be organised by our minds - and this means largely by the linguistic systems in our minds. We cut nature up, organise it into concepts, and ascribe significances as we do, largely because we are parties to an agreement that holds throughout our speech community and is codified in the patterns of our language. The agreement is, of course, an implicit and unstated one, but its terms are absolutely obligatory; we cannot talk at all except by subscribing to the organisation and classification of data which the agreement decrees. ' Surprisingly, though, neither Sapir or Whorf made it very clear whether they were arguing for strong or weak determinism. At times we are "at the mercy of" whatever language we speak, while at others our linguistic habits simply "predispose certain choices of interpretation". Whorf, originally a 'fire prevention engineer ' by trade, spent a lot of his time studying the language of the Hopi Indians of Arizona, who make no distinction in their language between past, present and future tenses; where in English it seems natural to distinguish between 'I see the girl ', 'I saw the girl ' and 'I will see the girl ', this is not an option in Hopi. This apparently made quite an impression on Whorf, who imagined that the scientists of the day and the Hopi must see the world very differently...although the philosopher Max Black considers that 'they may be expected to have pretty much the same concept of time that we have ' in spite of this. And Whorf himself notices, 'The Hopi language is capable of accounting for and describing correctly all observable phenomena of the universe '. Another characteristic of the Hopi tongue is that there is just a single word - 'masa 'ytaka ' - for everything that flies, including insects, aeroplanes and pilots.

Freud 'The question 'How does a thing become conscious? ' could be put more advantageously thus: 'How does a thing become pre-conscious? '. And the answer would be: 'By coming into connexion with the verbal images that correspond to it '. This quotation from Freud 's book 'The Ego and the Id ' helps us make what I consider to be a helpful distinction when talking about the influence of language on thought: whether we are talking about conscious or unconscious thought. I have suspected for a long time that language actually gives rise to

consciousness, to thought that is available to conscious introspection; thought of an unconscious nature takes place, I believe, from the day we are born, as the cognitive faculties exercise themselves upon the world of the child. But it is only when the child learns the meaning of words, learns to associate them with concepts, that he or she becomes 'conscious ', in the sense of becoming aware of his/her existence as the object of other 's thoughts and judgements, and exercising upon him/herself the internalised critic Freud calls the Superego. The child learns the words 'good ' and 'bad '; thought processes become their own objects for the first time. I think perhaps the answer might be that conscious thought is thought that has been given a verbal symbol to coexist alongside it. Thus thought that occurs below a conscious level, both the 'simple ' thought of cognitive processes and the complex thought of say, repressed ideas and affects, remains unconscious until verbal correspondences are found. More importantly, conscious thought may be thought of as unconscious thought that has been given access to consciousness through the use of verbal symbolia; thus words bring concepts from the conscious mind into the unconscious. But there is a price to be paid: what I believe to be an unlimited variety of concepts that could be brought to consciousness have but a limited number of words in which to clothe themselves. This, of course, relates to the question of whether language determines thought. I think it fair to say in the light of Freud 's theory, which seems to me to be undoubtedly correct, that yes, language does determine conscious thought, for conscious thought is by Freud 's definition thought that has been made conscious through language; but since the majority of thought is unquestionably unconscious, we cannot say that language determines thought wholly.

Conclusion
As regards linguistic determinism, it seems that most contemporary thinkers are quite content to accept the weaker version of the theory, that thought is indeed influenced by the linguistic systems available to us, but not much more; certainly not there are not many linguists today who would support Wilhelm von Humboldt 's 'Weltanschauung ' hypothesis. It can hardly be argued, either, that there is any limit to the structural diversity of languages. There are plenty of languages available for us to study, and each one divides the world up into compartments in different ways from other languages. To me it seems as if it would be profitable if some thought were given to the link between language and consciousness, the conscious coding of thought via verbal symbols and the way in which conscious thought is encoded in them.

Bibliography
Black, M.1962. Models and Metaphors. New York: Cornell University Press. Brown, R.1958. Words and Things. Illinois: The Free Press. Brown, Roger L.1968. Wilhelm von Humboldt 's Conception of Linguistic Relativity. Paris: Mouton. Ellis, A. and Beattie, G.1986. The Psychology of Language and Communication. New York: Guilford Press. Freud, S.1927. The Ego and the Id. London: The Hogarth Press. Lyons, J.1981. Language and Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Penn, J.1972. Linguistic Relativity versus Innate Ideas. Paris: Mouton. Rossi-Landi, F.1973. Ideologies of Linguistic Relativity. Paris: Mouton. Slobin, D.1974. Psycholinguistics. London: Scott, Foresman and Company.

Bibliography: Black, M.1962. Models and Metaphors. New York: Cornell University Press. Brown, R.1958. Words and Things. Illinois: The Free Press. Brown, Roger L.1968. Wilhelm von Humboldt 's Conception of Linguistic Relativity. Paris: Mouton. Ellis, A. and Beattie, G.1986. The Psychology of Language and Communication. New York: Guilford Press. Freud, S.1927. The Ego and the Id. London: The Hogarth Press. Lyons, J.1981. Language and Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Penn, J.1972. Linguistic Relativity versus Innate Ideas. Paris: Mouton. Rossi-Landi, F.1973. Ideologies of Linguistic Relativity. Paris: Mouton. Slobin, D.1974. Psycholinguistics. London: Scott, Foresman and Company.

You May Also Find These Documents Helpful

  • Good Essays

    If asked which cognitive ability you would miss the most if it were taken away, the majority of people would respond with the obvious choices of sight or hearing, but how many people would think about our sense of language? Language affects our lives in ways that we do not often realize. In the essay “How Language Shapes Thought” Lera Boroditsky argues that many of our cognitive abilities are enhanced, or hindered depending on the fundamental structure of our system of language. I found that Boroditsky used much of her own research in order to support her claims that direction, time and gender are concepts largely affected by the structural system of our language. Overall I found Boroditsky’s arguments to be sound and thorough. I agree with her claims that language shapes thought.…

    • 944 Words
    • 4 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Satisfactory Essays

    Based on Ralph Strauch in his book The Reality Illusion, some languages are structured around quite different basic word, such as categories and relationships. As a result, they project very different pictures of the basic nature of reality. For instance, the language of the Nootka Indians in the Pacific Northwest, has only one principle word-category which it denotes events or happening. Then, the Nootka perceive the world as a stream of transient events, rather than as the collection of less or more permanent objects which we see. Nobel Prize the winning physicist Werner Heisenberg said that things we are observing is not nature itself, but it is actually the nature exposed to our method of questioning. Language is the things that we depend…

    • 231 Words
    • 1 Page
    Satisfactory Essays
  • Good Essays

    This position is mainly associated with two American linguists, Edward Sapir (1884-1939) and Benjamin Lee Whorf (1897-1941). The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis as it is usually known, is that language actually determines thought. This theory is described as linguistic determinism.…

    • 2599 Words
    • 11 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Satisfactory Essays

    com week 1 quiz

    • 462 Words
    • 8 Pages

    A linguistic theory which states that language influences or shapes the speak­er’s ideas and views of the world is:…

    • 462 Words
    • 8 Pages
    Satisfactory Essays
  • Better Essays

    Emerson, R.W. (2009, September 9). Chapter IV: language. from Nature; Addresses and Lectures. Retrieved September 15, 2014, from http://www.emersoncentral.com/language.htm…

    • 1402 Words
    • 6 Pages
    Better Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    AP Psychology

    • 1534 Words
    • 7 Pages

    (Native Theorist) Inherent Existence of sets of cognitive structures Whorf- Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis Washoe, Sara and Koko- Ape language studies Jung- Collective unconscious; archetypes; Psychoanalytic…

    • 1534 Words
    • 7 Pages
    Powerful Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    Sapir states that "language is a guide to social reality" and that it "powerfully conditions all our thinking" (209). The language we speak conditions our social behavior and how we speak that language will affect our view of reality.…

    • 3301 Words
    • 14 Pages
    Powerful Essays
  • Good Essays

    The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis (SWH) states that there is a systematic relationship between the grammatical categories of the language a person speaks and how that person both understands the world and behaves in it. (Wikipedia) The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis was created by two American linguists, Edward Sapir and his student Edward Lee Whorf, in the early 1930s. It is considered to be a mould language theory, which represents language as a mould in terms of which thought categories are cast. (Bruner et al. 1956). At a very basic level, the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis consists of two linked ideas, that of linguistic relativity, where the language you speak will influence your outlook on the real world, and a stronger idea of linguistic determination, where our thinking and interpretation of the world around us is established by the language we speak.…

    • 1035 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis

    • 2753 Words
    • 12 Pages

    1. The Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis meant the comprehension of the world for an individual determines by the background linguistics system or grammar. In other words, language which use in his or her culture influence his or her thought, idea, view of the world. Explanation of this hypothesis will be more apprehensible by examples.…

    • 2753 Words
    • 12 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Good Essays

    target” ( Lutz 226,227 ). Language could change the image of reality completely if proper…

    • 1322 Words
    • 6 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    • Linguistic Benjamin Lee Whorf’s Linguistic determinism states language determines how we think. This is most evident in polylinguals (speaking 2 or more languages). I.e. someone who speaks English and Chinese will feel differently depending on which language they are using. English has many words describing personal emotions and Chinese has many words describing inter-personal emotions.…

    • 1206 Words
    • 5 Pages
    Powerful Essays
  • Good Essays

    rene margritte

    • 597 Words
    • 3 Pages

    Unquestionably, our knowledge of language and how we relate text between images is how we as human keep at a particular relationship with the world. Language is a system of representation; it functions in our world as a system of classification that helps us to understand the world we live in and also one another. According to Erwin Panofsky, Fernando de Saussure who was considered the founder of linguistics “defined the human as a language maker”3, this then lead Saussure accompanied alongside Charles S .Pierce to bring to life the system of semiotics or semiology.”Semiotic theory…focuses on communication as a social process” (Chaplin, E.)4. This distinction was made by the relationship between the signifier (word) and signified…

    • 597 Words
    • 3 Pages
    Good Essays
  • Better Essays

    Language can be looked at differently from other types of cognitions. There is a need for language in one form or another to have the ability to communicate with other human beings. This communication is the basis to how human beings express themselves to those around them. With this expression comes the ability to formulate thoughts. These thoughts can be translated to others through language. This language play an important role when analyzing, problem-solving, creating reasons, communicating needs, and making plans. Without the existence of language the attempt for humans to achieve goals would be almost impossible to accomplish. Goals would have to be accomplished be figuring out an alternative method than language to be used for sciences, history, mathematics, and the ability to explain past experiences or cultures. Because language is such an important communication tool, this paper will go into the definition of language and lexicon, evaluating the key features of language, with a description of the four levels of the language structure and processing, and analyzing the role of language processing in cognitive psychology.…

    • 1543 Words
    • 5 Pages
    Better Essays
  • Best Essays

    Reflective Essay

    • 2131 Words
    • 9 Pages

    • Vygotsky, L.S (1986) Thought and language (A.Kozulin trans) Cambridge, MA: MIT press (Original work published 1934)…

    • 2131 Words
    • 9 Pages
    Best Essays
  • Powerful Essays

    Psychology (Division of Neuropsychology of Language and Deafness), National Research Council (CNR), Rome, February . ———.  . Sette poesie in Lingua Italiana dei Segni (LIS). CD-ROM. Institute of Psychology, National Research Council (CNR), Rome. Givon, T. . Mind, Code, and Context: Essays in Pragmatics. Hillsdale, ´ N.J.: Erlbaum. Grady, J., T. Oakley, and S. Coulson. . Conceptual Blending and Metaphor. In Metaphors in Cognitive Linguistics, ed. G. Steen and R. Gibbs, –. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Johnston, T., and A. Schembri. . On Defining Lexeme in a Sign Language. Sign Language and Linguistics (): –. Keysar, B., and S. Glucksberg. . Understanding Metaphorical Comparisons. Psychological Review : –. Kittay, E., and A. Lehrer. . Semantic Fields and the Structure of Metaphor. Studies in Language : –. Klima, E. S., and U. Bellugi. . The Signs of Language. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Lakoff, G. . Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Lakoff, G., and M. Johnson. . Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ———. . Philosophy in the Flesh. New York: Basic Books. Lakoff, G., and M. Turner. . More Than Cool Reason: A Field Guide to Poetic Metaphor. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Ormsby, A. . Poetic Cohesion in ASL: Valli’s ‘‘Snowflake’’ and Coleridge’s ‘‘Frost at Midnight.’’ Sign Language Studies : –. Ortony, A. . The Role of Similarity in Similes and Metaphor. In Metaphor and Thought, ed. A. Ortony, –. New York: Cambridge University Press. ———, ed. . Metaphor and Thought. New York: Cambridge University Press. Peirce, C. S. –. Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce, ed. C. Hartshorne and P. Weiss. Cambridge: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. Pietrandrea, P. . Iconicity and Arbitrariness in Italian Sign Language. Sign Language Studies (): –. Pizzuto, E., E. Cameracanna, S. Corazza, and V. Volterra. . Terms for Spatio-temporal Relations in Italian Sign Language (LIS). In Iconicity in Language, ed. R. Simone, –. New York and Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Pizzuto, E., and V. Volterra. . Iconicity and Transparency in Sign Languages: A Crosslinguistic, Cross-cultural View. In The Signs of Language…

    • 6474 Words
    • 26 Pages
    Powerful Essays