Neural Marketing: Brain Response to Culturally Familiar Drinks

Topics: Brain, Cerebrum, Prefrontal cortex Pages: 20 (7568 words) Published: August 17, 2010
Neuron, Vol. 44, 379–387, October 14, 2004, Copyright 2004 by Cell Press

Neural Correlates of Behavioral Preference for Culturally Familiar Drinks Samuel M. McClure,1,2 Jian Li,1 Damon Tomlin, ´ Kim S. Cypert, Latane M. Montague, and P. Read Montague* Department of Neuroscience Menninger Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences Baylor College of Medicine 1 Baylor Plaza Houston, Texas 77030 neural responses, and the modulation of both by nonodor or nonflavor stimuli—that is, the sensory problem. Ultimately, such sensory discriminations and the variables that influence them serve to influence expressed behavioral preferences. Hence, there is another large piece of the problem to understand. For modern humans, behavioral preferences for food and beverages are potentially modulated by an enormous number of sensory variables, hedonic states, expectations, semantic priming, and social context. This assertion can be illustrated with a quote from Anderson and Sobel (2003) profiling the work of Small et al. (2003) on taste intensity and pleasantness processing: “A salad of perfectly grilled woodsy-flavored calamari paired with subtly bitter pale green leaves of curly endive and succulent petals of tomato flesh in a deep, rich balsamic dressing. Delicate slices of pan-roasted duck breast saturated with an assertive, tart-sweet tamarind-infused marinade.”

Summary Coca-Cola (Coke ) and Pepsi are nearly identical in chemical composition, yet humans routinely display strong subjective preferences for one or the other. This simple observation raises the important question of how cultural messages combine with content to shape our perceptions; even to the point of modifying behavioral preferences for a primary reward like a sugared drink. We delivered Coke and Pepsi to human subjects in behavioral taste tests and also in passive experiments carried out during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Two conditions were examined: (1) anonymous delivery of Coke and Pepsi and (2) brand-cued delivery of Coke and Pepsi. For the anonymous task, we report a consistent neural response in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex that correlated with subjects’ behavioral preferences for these beverages. In the brand-cued experiment, brand knowledge for one of the drinks had a dramatic influence on expressed behavioral preferences and on the measured brain responses. Introduction Perceptual constructs are generally multidimensional, integrating multiple physical and cognitive dimensions to generate coherent behavioral preferences. In sensory processing, the idea of multidimensional integration has long been used to frame a range of questions about cross-modal interactions in physiological and behavioral responses (Stein et al., 1996; 1999; Wallace and Stein, 1997; Armony and Dolan, 2001; Dolan et al., 2001; Laurienti et al., 2002, 2003). This same multidimensional perspective has also been developed for olfactory and gustatory processing, where the detection, discrimination, and perceived intensity of stimuli are not only functions of the primary physical properties (odors, flavors) but are also modulated “cross-modally” by visual input (Gottfried and Dolan, 2003), auditory input, and current reward value (Gottfried et al., 2003). The work just described has focused on the perceptual discrimination of odors and flavors, the correlated *Correspondence: read@bcm.tmc.edu 1 These authors contributed equally to this work. 2 Present address: Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey 08544.

The text goes on further, but note that the sheer lushness of the description adds somehow to the appeal of the food described. Also notice one implicit point of the description: many levels of social, cognitive, and cultural influences combine to produce behavioral preferences for food and drink. The above description likely would not appeal to a strict vegan or an owner of a pet duck. Anderson and Sobel point out that the...

References: Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C.F., O’Connor, E., and Breiter, H.C. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron 32, 537–551. Anderson, A.K., and Sobel, N. (2003). Dissociating intensity from valence as sensory inputs to emotion. Neuron 39, 581–583. Armony, J.L., and Dolan, R.J. (2001). Modulation of auditory neural responses by a visual context in human fear conditioning. Neuroreport 12, 3407–3411. Bechara, A., Damasio, A.R., Damasio, H., and Anderson, S.W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50, 7–15. Berns, G.S., McClure, S.M., Pagnoni, G., and Montague, P.R. (2001). Predictability modulates human brain response to reward. J. Neurosci. 21, 2793–2798. Chacko, E., McDuff, I., and Jackson, R. (2003). Replacing sugarbased soft drinks with sugar-free alternatives could slow the progress of the obesity epidemic: have your Coke and drink it too. N. Z. Med. J. 116, U649. Christian, K.M., and Thompson, R.F. (2003). Neural substrates of eyeblink conditioning: acquisition and retention. Learn. Mem. 10, 427–455. Davidson, R.J. (2002). Anxiety and affective style: role of prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Biol. Psychiatry 51, 68–80. Davidson, R.J., and Irwin, W. (1999). The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 11–21. Dolan, R.J., De Gleder, B., and Morris, J.S. (2001). Cross-modal binding of fear in voice and face. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98, 10006–10010. Eichenbaum, H. (2000). A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 41–50. Erk, S., Spitzer, M., Wunderlich, A.P., Galley, L., and Walter, H. (2002). Cultural objects modulate reward circuitry. Neuroreport 13, 2499–2503. Ford, E.S., Mokdad, A.H., and Giles, W.H. (2003). Trends in waist circumference among U.S. adults. Obes. Res. 11, 1223–1231. Forman, S.D., Cohen, J.D., Fitzgerald, M., Eddy, W.F., Mintun, M.A., and Noll, D.C. (1995). Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a clustersize threshold. Magn. Reson. Med. 33, 636–647. Friston, K.J., Holmes, A.P., Worsley, K., Poline, J.P., Frith, C.D., and Frackowiak, R.S.J. (1995). Statistical parametric maps in functional brain imaging: a general linear approach. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 189–210.
Brain Response to Culturally Familiar Drinks 387
Genovese, C.R., Lazar, N.A., and Nichols, T. (2002). Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage 15, 870–878. Gottfried, J.A., and Dolan, R.J. (2003). The nose smells what the eye sees: crossmodal visual facilitation of human olfactory perception. Neuron 39, 375–386. Gottfried, J.A., O’Doherty, J., and Dloan, R.J. (2003). Encoding predictive reward value in human amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Science 301, 1104–1107. Greene, J.D., Sommerville, R.B., Nystrom, L.E., Darley, J.M., and Cohen, J.D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 293, 2105–2108. Iidaka, T., Terashima, S., Yamashita, K., Okada, T., Sadato, N., and Yonekura, Y. (2003). Dissociable neural responses in the hippocampus to the retrieval of facial identity and emotion: an event-related fMRI study. Hippocampus 13, 429–436. Knutson, B., Adams, C.M., Fong, G.W., and Hommer, D. (2001). Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. J. Neurosci. 21, RC159. Laurienti, P.J., Burdette, J.H., Wallace, M.T., Yen, Y.-F., Field, A.S., and Stein, B.E. (2002). Activity in visual and auditory cortex can be modulated by influences from multiple senses. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 420–429. Laurienti, P.J., Wallace, M.T., Maldjian, J.A., Susi, C.M., Stein, B.E., and Burdette, J.H. (2003). Cross-modal sensory processing in the anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortices. Hum. Brain Mapp. 19, 213–223. McClure, S.M., Berns, G.S., and Montague, P.R. (2003). Temporal prediction errors in a passive learning task activate human striatum. Neuron 38, 339–346. McEchron, M.D., and Disterhoft, J.F. (1999). Hippocampal encoding of non-spatial trace conditioning. Hippocampus 9, 385–396. Markowitsch, H.J., Vandekerckhovel, M.M., Lanfermann, H., and Russ, M.O. (2003). Engagement of lateral and medial prefrontal areas in the ecphory of sad and happy autobiographical memories. Cortex 39, 643–665. Mineka, S., Watson, D., and Clark, L.A. (1998). Comorbidity of anxiety and unipolar mood disorders. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 49, 377–412. Ochsner, K.N., Bunge, S.A., Gross, J.J., and Gabrieli, J.D. (2002). Rethinking feelings: an FMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 1215–1229. O’Doherty, J.P., Dayan, P., Friston, K.J., Critchley, H., and Dolan, R.J. (2003a). Temporal difference models and reward-related learning in the human brain. Neuron 38, 329–337. O’Doherty, J.P., Critchley, H., Deichmann, R., and Dolan, R.J. (2003b). Dissociating valence of outcome from behavioral control in human orbital and ventral prefrontal cortices. J. Neurosci. 23, 7931–7939. Popkin, B.M., and Nielsen, S.J. (2003). The sweetening of the world’s diet. Obes. Res. 11, 1325–1332. Sanfey, A.G., Rilling, J.K., Aronson, J.A., Nystrom, L.E., and Cohen, J.D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game. Science 300, 1755–1758. Schall, J.D., Stuphorn, V., and Brown, J.W. (2002). Monitoring and control of action by the frontal lobes. Neuron 36, 309–322. Small, D.M., Gregory, M.D., Mak, Y.E., Gitelman, D., Mesulam, M.M., and Parrish, T. (2003). Dissociation of neural representation of intensity and affective valuation in human gustation. Neuron 39, 701–711. Stein, B.E., London, N., Wilkinson, L.K., and Price, D.D. (1996). Enhancement of perceived visual intensity by auditory stimuli: A psychophysical analysis. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 497–506. Stein, B.E., Wallace, M.T., and Stanford, T.R. (1999). Development of multisensory integration: transforming sensory input into motor output. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 5, 72–85. Wallace, M.T., and Stein, B.E. (1997). Development of multi-sensory integration in cat superior colliculus. J. Neurosci. 17, 2429–2444. Watanabe, M. (1996). Reward expectancy in primate prefrontal neurons. Nature 382, 629–632.
Wyatt, H.R. (2003). The prevalence of obesity. Prim. Care 30, 267–279. Zimmet, P. (2003). The burden of type 2 diabetes: are we doing enough? Diabetes. Metab. 29, 6S9–18. Zink, C.F., Pagnoni, G., Martin-Skurski, M.E., Chappelow, J.C., and Berns, G.S. (2004). Human striatal responses to monetary reward depend on saliency. Neuron 42, 509–517.
Continue Reading

Please join StudyMode to read the full document

You May Also Find These Documents Helpful

  • Essay on Marketing
  • Morality is a culturally conditioned response Essay
  • Marketing Essay
  • Beh 225 Brain Response of Behavior Essay
  • Essay on marketing
  • Essay on Marketing
  • Marketing Essay
  • Marketing Essay

Become a StudyMode Member

Sign Up - It's Free